Analytics: Difference between revisions
mNo edit summary |
mNo edit summary |
||
Line 1: | Line 1: | ||
Explain how the Jungian approach of Analytics is used all throughout the research, however, this segment is an appendix into synchronicity. | |||
'''Synchronicity''' is a concept developed by '''Carl Jung''' to describe '''meaningful coincidences''' that occur without causal connection but are related through their '''shared meaning'''. Jung defined synchronicity as the '''"acausal connection of two or more events"''', where inner psychological states and external events align in a manner that transcends conventional causality. | |||
Jung proposed that these occurrences reflect a deeper '''order within the universe''', often linked to the '''collective unconscious''', a shared repository of archetypes and experiences inherent to all human beings. Synchronicity, in this context, serves as a mechanism by which the '''unconscious mind''' communicates with the external world, facilitating '''self-discovery''' and '''psychological growth'''. These meaningful coincidences, often occurring at significant moments, can guide individuals in their '''journey of individuation''', the process by which one integrates unconscious aspects of the psyche into a more complete sense of self. Jung's theory of synchronicity suggests that '''events''' are connected in ways beyond mere '''cause and effect''', offering profound insight into the '''interconnectedness of consciousness''', the '''unconscious mind''', and the '''broader cosmos'''. | |||
=== Omens === | |||
<blockquote>Ancient philosophers and physicians believed a human mind to be a collection of mental faculties. They divided the mind, not with an understanding of biology or the brain, but to capture the essence of human nature according to their concerns about truth, beauty and ethics. The faculties in question have morphed over the millennia, but generally speaking, they encompass mental categories for thinking (cognitions), feeling (emotions) and volition (actions, and in more modern versions, perceptions). These mental categories symbolize a cherished narrative about human nature in Western civilization: that emotions (our inner beast) and cognitions (evolution’s crowning achievement) battle or cooperate to control behavior.<sup>1</sup> The classical view of emotion (Figure 1) was forged in these ancient ideas. Affective neuroscience takes its inspiration from this faculty-based approach. Scientists begin with emotion concepts that are most recognizably English (Pavlenko, 2014; Wierzbicka, 2014), such as anger, sadness, fear, and disgust, and search for their elusive biological essences (i.e. their neural signatures or fingerprints), usually in subcortical regions. This inductive approach assumes that the emotion categories we experience and perceive as distinct must also be distinct in nature. If the history of science has taught us anything, however, it is that human experiences rarely reveal the way that the natural world works: ‘Physical concepts are free creations of the human mind, and are not; however, it may seem, uniquely determined by the external world’ (Einstein ''et al.'', 1938, p. 33). The last two decades of neuroscience research have brought us to the brink of a paradigm shift in understanding the workings of the brain, setting the stage to revolutionize our study of emotions (or any mental category). So in this article, we turn the typical inductive approach on its head. We begin not with mental categories but with the structure and function of the brain, and from there deduce what the biological basis of emotions might be. The answer, I suggest, will look something like the theory of constructed emotion (Barrett, 2017), formerly, the conceptual act theory of emotion (Barrett, 2006b, 2011a, 2012, 2013, 2014). [https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC5390700/#nsw154-B218 The theory of constructed emotion: an active inference account of interoception and categorization]</blockquote>The importance of using grounding arguments in defining emotions is paramount, [[wikipedia:Interoception|interoception]] is used on current neuroscientific research to explain how emotions emerge from a source of [[wikipedia:Anatomy|Anatomical]] organization. Albeit contrary to what posited in the [[Main Page#Language, Cognition, and Symbolic Evolution|initial chapters of '''''Zeus''''']], it is of belief both can be [https://www.therapyroute.com/article/what-is-emotion-by-m-solms complimentary] in shaping the state of emotions to humans. Omens are regarded as the emergent aspects of emotions that cannot be controlled by the body and its subsystems anatomy. | |||
In order of importance: | In order of importance: | ||
=== Luck === | ==== Luck ==== | ||
Lorem | Lorem | ||
=== Creativity === | ==== Creativity ==== | ||
Lorem | Lorem | ||
=== Synchronicity === | ==== Synchronicity ==== | ||
Lorem | Lorem | ||
=== Intuition === | ==== Intuition ==== | ||
Lorem | Lorem | ||
=== Beauty === | ==== Beauty ==== | ||
Lorem | Lorem |
Revision as of 00:17, 19 March 2025
Explain how the Jungian approach of Analytics is used all throughout the research, however, this segment is an appendix into synchronicity.
Synchronicity is a concept developed by Carl Jung to describe meaningful coincidences that occur without causal connection but are related through their shared meaning. Jung defined synchronicity as the "acausal connection of two or more events", where inner psychological states and external events align in a manner that transcends conventional causality.
Jung proposed that these occurrences reflect a deeper order within the universe, often linked to the collective unconscious, a shared repository of archetypes and experiences inherent to all human beings. Synchronicity, in this context, serves as a mechanism by which the unconscious mind communicates with the external world, facilitating self-discovery and psychological growth. These meaningful coincidences, often occurring at significant moments, can guide individuals in their journey of individuation, the process by which one integrates unconscious aspects of the psyche into a more complete sense of self. Jung's theory of synchronicity suggests that events are connected in ways beyond mere cause and effect, offering profound insight into the interconnectedness of consciousness, the unconscious mind, and the broader cosmos.
Omens
Ancient philosophers and physicians believed a human mind to be a collection of mental faculties. They divided the mind, not with an understanding of biology or the brain, but to capture the essence of human nature according to their concerns about truth, beauty and ethics. The faculties in question have morphed over the millennia, but generally speaking, they encompass mental categories for thinking (cognitions), feeling (emotions) and volition (actions, and in more modern versions, perceptions). These mental categories symbolize a cherished narrative about human nature in Western civilization: that emotions (our inner beast) and cognitions (evolution’s crowning achievement) battle or cooperate to control behavior.1 The classical view of emotion (Figure 1) was forged in these ancient ideas. Affective neuroscience takes its inspiration from this faculty-based approach. Scientists begin with emotion concepts that are most recognizably English (Pavlenko, 2014; Wierzbicka, 2014), such as anger, sadness, fear, and disgust, and search for their elusive biological essences (i.e. their neural signatures or fingerprints), usually in subcortical regions. This inductive approach assumes that the emotion categories we experience and perceive as distinct must also be distinct in nature. If the history of science has taught us anything, however, it is that human experiences rarely reveal the way that the natural world works: ‘Physical concepts are free creations of the human mind, and are not; however, it may seem, uniquely determined by the external world’ (Einstein et al., 1938, p. 33). The last two decades of neuroscience research have brought us to the brink of a paradigm shift in understanding the workings of the brain, setting the stage to revolutionize our study of emotions (or any mental category). So in this article, we turn the typical inductive approach on its head. We begin not with mental categories but with the structure and function of the brain, and from there deduce what the biological basis of emotions might be. The answer, I suggest, will look something like the theory of constructed emotion (Barrett, 2017), formerly, the conceptual act theory of emotion (Barrett, 2006b, 2011a, 2012, 2013, 2014). The theory of constructed emotion: an active inference account of interoception and categorization
The importance of using grounding arguments in defining emotions is paramount, interoception is used on current neuroscientific research to explain how emotions emerge from a source of Anatomical organization. Albeit contrary to what posited in the initial chapters of Zeus, it is of belief both can be complimentary in shaping the state of emotions to humans. Omens are regarded as the emergent aspects of emotions that cannot be controlled by the body and its subsystems anatomy.
In order of importance:
Luck
Lorem
Creativity
Lorem
Synchronicity
Lorem
Intuition
Lorem
Beauty
Lorem